Item request has been placed!
×
Item request cannot be made.
×

Different types of focus: Caregiver-child interaction and changes in preschool children's attention in two cultures.
Item request has been placed!
×
Item request cannot be made.
×

- Author(s): Senzaki S;Senzaki S; Shimizu Y; Shimizu Y
- Source:
Child development [Child Dev] 2022 May; Vol. 93 (3), pp. e348-e356. Date of Electronic Publication: 2022 Jan 31.- Publication Type:
Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't; Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural- Language:
English - Source:
- Additional Information
- Source: Publisher: Blackwell Publishers Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 0372725 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1467-8624 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 00093920 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Child Dev Subsets: MEDLINE
- Publication Information: Publication: Malden, MA : Blackwell Publishers
Original Publication: [Chicago, etc.] : Published by the University of Chicago Press for the Society for Research in Child Development [etc.] - Subject Terms:
- Abstract: Social contexts shape the development of attention; however, little is known about joint attention beyond infancy. This study employed behavioral and eye-tracking measurements to investigate cultural variations in how caregivers direct 3- to 4-year-old children's attention and subsequent changes in children's attention to objects and contextual backgrounds in the United States (predominantly non-Hispanic Whites) and Japan (N = 60 mother-child dyads, 29 girls, 31 boys). The findings revealed that caregivers directed children's attention to culturally sensitive information, and significant cross-cultural differences in attention emerged after caregiver-child interaction, with Japanese children shifting their attention to the backgrounds. Results provide new insights into the role of social interaction and cultural diversity in the development of attention.
(© 2022 The Authors. Child Development © 2022 Society for Research in Child Development.) - References: Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during Childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71-82. https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.
Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716-724. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x.
Bates, D., Mchler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1-48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.
Booth, A. E., Shavlik, M., & Haden, C. A. (2020). Parents’ causal talk: Links to children’s causal stance and emerging scientific literacy. Developmental Psychology, 56, 2055-2064. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001108.
Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Haynes, O. M., Suwalsky, J. T., & Bakeman, R. (2012). Modalities of infant-mother interaction in Japanese, Japanese American immigrant, and European American dyads. Child Development, 83, 2073-2088. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01822.x.
Chavajay, P., & Rogoff, B. (2002). Schooling and traditional collaborative social organization of problem solving by Mayan mothers and children. Developmental Psychology, 38, 55-66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.55.
Chen, K., Cheng, Y., Berkout, O., & Lindhiem, O. (2017). Analyzing proportion scores as outcomes for prevention trials: A Statistical primer. Prevention Science, 18, 312-321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0643-6.
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 12629-12633. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102.
Cohen, D., & Kitayama, S. (2020). Handbook of cultural psychology, 2nd ed. Guilford Press.
Correa-Chávez, M., Rogoff, B., & Mejía Arauz, R. (2005). Cultural patterns in attending to two events at once. Child Development, 76, 664-678. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00870.x.
Darby, K. P., Deng, S. W., Walther, D. B., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2020). The development of attention to objects and scenes: From object biased to unbiased. Child Development, 92, 1173-1186. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13469.
Dink, J. W., & Ferguson, B. (2015). eyetrackingR: An R library for eye-tracking data analysis. http://www.eyetrackingr.com.
Duffy, S., Toriyama, R., Itakura, S., & Kitayama, S. (2009). The development of culturally-contingent attention strategies in young children in the U.S. and Japan. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 351-359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.06.006.
Egami, C., Morita, K., Ohya, T., Ishii, Y., Yamashita, Y., & Matsuishi, T. (2009). Developmental characteristics of visual cognitive function during childhood according to exploratory eye movements. Brain & Development, 31, 750-757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2008.12.002.
Fernald, A., & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers’ speech to infants. Child Development, 64, 637-656. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.
Fivush, R., Marin, K., McWilliams, K., & Bohanek, J. G. (2009). Family reminiscing style: Parent gender and emotional focus in relation to child well-being. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 210-235. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370903155866.
Gathercole, V., & Min, H. (1997). Word meaning biases or language-specific effects? Evidence from English, Spanish and Korean. First Language, 17, 31-56.
Gauvain, M. (2002). The social context of cognitive development. Guilford.
Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., & Raver, J. L. (2006). On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1225-1244. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1225.
Gelman, S. A., & Legare, C. H. (2011). Concepts and folk theories. Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 379-398. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-081309-145822.
Hamaguchi, E. (1985). A contextual model of the Japanese: Toward a methodological innovation in Japan studies. Journal of Japanese Studies, 11-2, 289-321.
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19, 12-17. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.x.
Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360-1366. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1146282.
Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., & Itakura, S. (2013). East-West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science, 16, 198-208. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12016.
Imai, M., & Mazuka, R. (2007). Language-relative construal of individuation constrained by universal ontology: Revisiting language universals and linguistic relativity. Cognitive Science, 31, 385-413. https://doi.org/10.1080/15326900701326436.
Ishii, K., Rule, N. O., & Toriyama, R. (2017). Context sensitivity in Canadian and Japanese children's judgments of emotion. Current Psychology, https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9446-y.
Keller, H. (2017). Culture and development: A systematic relationship. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 833-840. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617704097.
Köster, M., Itakura, S., Yovsi, R., & Kärtner, J. (2018). Visual attention in 5-year-olds from three different cultures. PLoS One, 13, e020023. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200239.
Köster, M., & Kärtner, J. (2018). Context-sensitive attention is socialized via a verbal route in the parent-child interaction. PLoS One, 13, e0207113. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207113.
Kuwabara, M., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Cross cultural differences in cognitive development: Attention to relations and objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 20-35.
Kuwabara, M., & Smith, L. B. (2016). Cultural differences in visual object recognition in 3-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 22-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.006.
Marin, K. A., Bohanek, J. G., & Fivush, R. (2008). Positive effects of talking about the negative: Family narratives of negative experiences and preadolescents’ perceived competence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 573-593. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2008.00572.x.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922.
Miller, J. G. (1997). Taking culture into account in social cognitive development. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, 9-34. https://doi.org/10.1177/097133369700900102.
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2020). The development of autobiographical memory, autobiographical narratives, and autobiographical consciousness. Psychological Reports, 123, 71-96. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294119852574.
Nielsen, M., & Haun, D. B. (2016). Why developmental psychology is incomplete without comparative and cross-cultural perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150071. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0071.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291-310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291.
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26, 653-684. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2605_4.
O'Hanlon, C. G., & Read, J. C. (2017). Blindness to background: An inbuilt bias for visual objects. Developmental Science, 20, e12478. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12478.
Over, H. (2016). The origins of belonging: Social motivation in infants and young children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150072.
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311-342. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717-731. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, v-vi, 1-174; discussion 175-9. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166109.
Senzaki, S., Masuda, T., & Ishii, K. (2014). When is perception top-down and when is it not? Culture, narrative, and attention. Cognitive Science, 38, 1493-1506. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12118.
Senzaki, S., Masuda, T., Takada, A., & Okada, H. (2016). The communication of culturally dominant modes of attention from parents to children: A comparison of Canadian and Japanese parent-child conversations during a joint scene description task. PLoS One, 11, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147199.
Senzaki, S., & Shimizu, Y. (2020). Early learning environments for the development of attention: Maternal narratives in the United States and Japan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 51, 187-202. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022120910804.
Shimizu, Y., Senzaki, S., & Cowell, J. M. (2021). Cultural similarities and differences in the development of sociomoral judgments: An eye-tracking study. Cognitive Development, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100974.
Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59, 1-38. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v059.i05.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Westview Press.
Varnum, M. E. W., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, E. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition: Evidence for the social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions of Psychological Science, 19, 9-13. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359301.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.
Wang, Q. (2001). Did you have fun? Cognitive Development, 16, 693-715. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00055-7.
Wang, Q. (2017). Putting culture in the middle in judging scholarly merit. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 1166-1170. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617724240.
Wang, Q. (2018). Studying cognitive development in cultural context: A multi-level analysis approach. Developmental Review, 50, 54-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.03.002.
Wass, S. V., Clackson, K., Georgieva, S. D., Brightman, L., Nutbrown, R., & Leong, V. (2018). Infants’ visual sustained attention is higher during joint play than solo play: Is this due to increased endogenous attention control or exogenous stimulus capture? Developmental Science, 21, e12667. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12667.
Waxman, S. R., Fu, X., Ferguson, B., Geraghty, K., Leddon, E., Liang, J., & Zhao, M.-F. (2016). How early is infants’ attention to objects and actions shaped by culture? New evidence from 24-month-olds raised in the U.S. and China. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1664-1078. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00097.
Yoshida, H., & Smith, L. B. (2008). Early noun lexicons in English and Japanese. Cognition, 82, 63-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00153-6.
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2016). The social origins of sustained attention in one-year-old human infants. Current Biology, 26, 1235-1240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.026.
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2017). Multiple sensory-motor pathways lead to coordinated visual attention. Cognitive Science, 5-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12366. - Grant Information: R15 HD094138 United States HD NICHD NIH HHS
- Publication Date: Date Created: 20220131 Date Completed: 20220608 Latest Revision: 20220816
- Publication Date: 20250114
- Accession Number: 10.1111/cdev.13731
- Accession Number: 35098526
- Source:

Copyright © Department of Culture and Tourism, all rights reserved.
Copyright © 2024 Department of Culture and Tourism, all rights reserved. Powered By EBSCO Stacks 3.3.0 [353] | Staff Login
No Comments.